petak, 7. lipnja 2013.

Ethan Watters - We Aren’t the World: Joe Henrich and his colleagues are shaking the foundations of psychology and economics



Kulturne razlike utječu na naše mišljenje, percepciju stvarnosti, ukuse i vrijednosti još dublje nego što se mislilo. Gledajući u planetarnom kontekstu, zapadnjaci su, posebice Amerikanci, zapravo marginalni čudaci pa svi "testovi" rađeni sa zapadnjacima ništa ne govore o ljudima "općenito".
Mnoštvo zanimljivih posljedica.



We Aren’t the World

Joe Henrich and his colleagues are shaking the foundations of psychology and economics—and hoping to change the way social scientists think about human behavior and culture.

(ILLUSTRATION: MARK MCGINNIS)
 
IN THE SUMMER of 1995, a young graduate student in anthropology at UCLA named Joe Henrich traveled to Peru to carry out some fieldwork among the Machiguenga, an indigenous people who live north of Machu Picchu in the Amazon basin. The Machiguenga had traditionally been horticulturalists who lived in single-family, thatch-roofed houses in small hamlets composed of clusters of extended families. For sustenance, they relied on local game and produce from small-scale farming. They shared with their kin but rarely traded with outside groups.
While the setting was fairly typical for an anthropologist, Henrich’s research was not. Rather than practice traditional ethnography, he decided to run a behavioral experiment that had been developed by economists. Henrich used a “game”—along the lines of the famous prisoner’s dilemma—to see whether isolated cultures shared with the West the same basic instinct for fairness. In doing so, Henrich expected to confirm one of the foundational assumptions underlying such experiments, and indeed underpinning the entire fields of economics and psychology: that humans all share the same cognitive machinery—the same evolved rational and psychological hardwiring.
The test that Henrich introduced to the Machiguenga was called the ultimatum game. The rules are simple: in each game there are two players who remain anonymous to each other. The first player is given an amount of money, say $100, and told that he has to offer some of the cash, in an amount of his choosing, to the other subject. The second player can accept or refuse the split. But there’s a hitch: players know that if the recipient refuses the offer, both leave empty-handed. North Americans, who are the most common subjects for such experiments, usually offer a 50-50 split when on the giving end. When on the receiving end, they show an eagerness to punish the other player for uneven splits at their own expense. In short, Americans show the tendency to be equitable with strangers—and to punish those who are not.
Among the Machiguenga, word quickly spread of the young, square-jawed visitor from America giving away money. The stakes Henrich used in the game with the Machiguenga were not insubstantial—roughly equivalent to the few days’ wages they sometimes earned from episodic work with logging or oil companies. So Henrich had no problem finding volunteers. What he had great difficulty with, however, was explaining the rules, as the game struck the Machiguenga as deeply odd.
When he began to run the game it became immediately clear that Machiguengan behavior was dramatically different from that of the average North American. To begin with, the offers from the first player were much lower. In addition, when on the receiving end of the game, the Machiguenga rarely refused even the lowest possible amount. “It just seemed ridiculous to the Machiguenga that you would reject an offer of free money,” says Henrich. “They just didn’t understand why anyone would sacrifice money to punish someone who had the good luck of getting to play the other role in the game.”
Joe Henrich was a graduate student when he tested the ultimatum game on the Machiguenga of Peru.
Joe Henrich and research assistant Vilisi adminster the Third Party Punishment Game in the village of Teci on Fiji’s Yasawa Island.
The potential implications of the unexpected results were quickly apparent to Henrich. He knew that a vast amount of scholarly literature in the social sciences—particularly in economics and psychology—relied on the ultimatum game and similar experiments. At the heart of most of that research was the implicit assumption that the results revealed evolved psychological traits common to all humans, never mind that the test subjects were nearly always from the industrialized West. Henrich realized that if the Machiguenga results stood up, and if similar differences could be measured across other populations, this assumption of universality would have to be challenged.
Henrich had thought he would be adding a small branch to an established tree of knowledge. It turned out he was sawing at the very trunk. He began to wonder: What other certainties about “human nature” in social science research would need to be reconsidered when tested across diverse populations?
Henrich soon landed a grant from the MacArthur Foundation to take his fairness games on the road. With the help of a dozen other colleagues he led a study of 14 other small-scale societies, in locales from Tanzania to Indonesia. Differences abounded in the behavior of both players in the ultimatum game. In no society did he find people who were purely selfish (that is, who always offered the lowest amount, and never refused a split), but average offers from place to place varied widely and, in some societies—ones where gift-giving is heavily used to curry favor or gain allegiance—the first player would often make overly generous offers in excess of 60 percent, and the second player would often reject them, behaviors almost never observed among Americans.
The research established Henrich as an up-and-coming scholar. In 2004, he was given the U.S. Presidential Early Career Award for young scientists at the White House. But his work also made him a controversial figure. When he presented his research to the anthropology department at the University of British Columbia during a job interview a year later, he recalls a hostile reception. Anthropology is the social science most interested in cultural differences, but the young scholar’s methods of using games and statistics to test and compare cultures with the West seemed heavy-handed and invasive to some. “Professors from the anthropology department suggested it was a bad thing that I was doing,” Henrich remembers. “The word ‘unethical’ came up.”
So instead of toeing the line, he switched teams. A few well-placed people at the University of British Columbia saw great promise in Henrich’s work and created a position for him, split between the economics department and the psychology department. It was in the psychology department that he found two kindred spirits in Steven Heine and Ara Norenzayan. Together the three set about writing a paper that they hoped would fundamentally challenge the way social scientists thought about human behavior, cognition, and culture.

A MODERN LIBERAL ARTS education gives lots of lip service to the idea of cultural diversity. It’s generally agreed that all of us see the world in ways that are sometimes socially and culturally constructed, that pluralism is good, and that ethnocentrism is bad. But beyond that the ideas get muddy. That we should welcome and celebrate people of all backgrounds seems obvious, but the implied corollary—that people from different ethno-cultural origins have particular attributes that add spice to the body politic—becomes more problematic. To avoid stereotyping, it is rarely stated bluntly just exactly what those culturally derived qualities might be. Challenge liberal arts graduates on their appreciation of cultural diversity and you’ll often find them retreating to the anodyne notion that under the skin everyone is really alike.
If you take a broad look at the social science curriculum of the last few decades, it becomes a little more clear why modern graduates are so unmoored. The last generation or two of undergraduates have largely been taught by a cohort of social scientists busily doing penance for the racism and Eurocentrism of their predecessors, albeit in different ways. Many anthropologists took to the navel gazing of postmodernism and swore off attempts at rationality and science, which were disparaged as weapons of cultural imperialism.
Economists and psychologists, for their part, did an end run around the issue with the convenient assumption that their job was to study the human mind stripped of culture. The human brain is genetically comparable around the globe, it was agreed, so human hardwiring for much behavior, perception, and cognition should be similarly universal. No need, in that case, to look beyond the convenient population of undergraduates for test subjects. A 2008 survey of the top six psychology journals dramatically shows how common that assumption was: more than 96 percent of the subjects tested in psychological studies from 2003 to 2007 were Westerners—with nearly 70 percent from the United States alone. Put another way: 96 percent of human subjects in these studies came from countries that represent only 12 percent of the world’s population.
Henrich’s work with the ultimatum game was an example of a small but growing countertrend in the social sciences, one in which researchers look straight at the question of how deeply culture shapes human cognition. His new colleagues in the psychology department, Heine and Norenzayan, were also part of this trend. Heine focused on the different ways people in Western and Eastern cultures perceived the world, reasoned, and understood themselves in relationship to others. Norenzayan’s research focused on the ways religious belief influenced bonding and behavior. The three began to compile examples of cross-cultural research that, like Henrich’s work with the Machiguenga, challenged long-held assumptions of human psychological universality.
Some of that research went back a generation. It was in the 1960s, for instance, that researchers discovered that aspects of visual perception were different from place to place. One of the classics of the literature, the Müller-Lyer illusion, showed that where you grew up would determine to what degree you would fall prey to the illusion that these two lines are different in length:
Muller Lyer Illusion Comparison 3
Researchers found that Americans perceive the line with the ends feathered outward (B) as being longer than the line with the arrow tips (A). San foragers of the Kalahari, on the other hand, were more likely to see the lines as they are: equal in length. Subjects from more than a dozen cultures were tested, and Americans were at the far end of the distribution—seeing the illusion more dramatically than all others.
More recently psychologists had challenged the universality of research done in the 1950s by pioneering social psychologist Solomon Asch. Asch had discovered that test subjects were often willing to make incorrect judgments on simple perception tests to conform with group pressure. When the test was performed across 17 societies, however, it turned out that group pressure had a range of influence. Americans were again at the far end of the scale, in this case showing the least tendency to conform to group belief.
As Heine, Norenzayan, and Henrich furthered their search, they began to find research suggesting wide cultural differences almost everywhere they looked: in spatial reasoning, the way we infer the motivations of others, categorization, moral reasoning, the boundaries between the self and others, and other arenas. These differences, they believed, were not genetic. The distinct ways Americans and Machiguengans played the ultimatum game, for instance, wasn’t because they had differently evolved brains. Rather, Americans, without fully realizing it, were manifesting a psychological tendency shared with people in other industrialized countries that had been refined and handed down through thousands of generations in ever more complex market economies. When people are constantly doing business with strangers, it helps when they have the desire to go out of their way (with a lawsuit, a call to the Better Business Bureau, or a bad Yelp review) when they feel cheated. Because Machiguengan culture had a different history, their gut feeling about what was fair was distinctly their own. In the small-scale societies with a strong culture of gift-giving, yet another conception of fairness prevailed. There, generous financial offers were turned down because people’s minds had been shaped by a cultural norm that taught them that the acceptance of generous gifts brought burdensome obligations. Our economies hadn’t been shaped by our sense of fairness; it was the other way around.
The growing body of cross-cultural research that the three researchers were compiling suggested that the mind’s capacity to mold itself to cultural and environmental settings was far greater than had been assumed. The most interesting thing about cultures may not be in the observable things they do—the rituals, eating preferences, codes of behavior, and the like—but in the way they mold our most fundamental conscious and unconscious thinking and perception.
For instance, the different ways people perceive the Müller-Lyer illusion likely reflects lifetimes spent in different physical environments. American children, for the most part, grow up in box-shaped rooms of varying dimensions. Surrounded by carpentered corners, visual perception adapts to this strange new environment (strange and new in terms of human history, that is) by learning to perceive converging lines in three dimensions.
When unconsciously translated in three dimensions, the line with the outward-feathered ends (C) appears farther away and the brain therefore judges it to be longer. The more time one spends in natural environments, where there are no carpentered corners, the less one sees the illusion.
As the three continued their work, they noticed something else that was remarkable: again and again one group of people appeared to be particularly unusual when compared to other populations—with perceptions, behaviors, and motivations that were almost always sliding down one end of the human bell curve.
In the end they titled their paper “The Weirdest People in the World?” (pdf) By “weird” they meant both unusual and Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic. It is not just our Western habits and cultural preferences that are different from the rest of the world, it appears. The very way we think about ourselves and others—and even the way we perceive reality—makes us distinct from other humans on the planet, not to mention from the vast majority of our ancestors. Among Westerners, the data showed that Americans were often the most unusual, leading the researchers to conclude that “American participants are exceptional even within the unusual population of Westerners—outliers among outliers.”
Given the data, they concluded that social scientists could not possibly have picked a worse population from which to draw broad generalizations. Researchers had been doing the equivalent of studying penguins while believing that they were learning insights applicable to all birds.

NOT LONG AGO I met Henrich, Heine, and Norenzayan for dinner at a small French restaurant in Vancouver, British Columbia, to hear about the reception of their weird paper, which was published in the prestigious journal Behavioral and Brain Sciences in 2010. The trio of researchers are young—as professors go—good-humored family men. They recalled that they were nervous as the publication time approached. The paper basically suggested that much of what social scientists thought they knew about fundamental aspects of human cognition was likely only true of one small slice of humanity. They were making such a broadside challenge to whole libraries of research that they steeled themselves to the possibility of becoming outcasts in their own fields.
“We were scared,” admitted Henrich. “We were warned that a lot of people were going to be upset.”
“We were told we were going to get spit on,” interjected Norenzayan.
“Yes,” Henrich said. “That we’d go to conferences and no one was going to sit next to us at lunchtime.”
Interestingly, they seemed much less concerned that they had used the pejorative acronym WEIRD to describe a significant slice of humanity, although they did admit that they could only have done so to describe their own group. “Really,” said Henrich, “the only people we could have called weird are represented right here at this table.”
Still, I had to wonder whether describing the Western mind, and the American mind in particular, as weird suggested that our cognition is not just different but somehow malformed or twisted. In their paper the trio pointed out cross-cultural studies that suggest that the “weird” Western mind is the most self-aggrandizing and egotistical on the planet: we are more likely to promote ourselves as individuals versus advancing as a group. WEIRD minds are also more analytic, possessing the tendency to telescope in on an object of interest rather than understanding that object in the context of what is around it.
The WEIRD mind also appears to be unique in terms of how it comes to understand and interact with the natural world. Studies show that Western urban children grow up so closed off in man-made environments that their brains never form a deep or complex connection to the natural world. While studying children from the U.S., researchers have suggested a developmental timeline for what is called “folkbiological reasoning.” These studies posit that it is not until children are around 7 years old that they stop projecting human qualities onto animals and begin to understand that humans are one animal among many. Compared to Yucatec Maya communities in Mexico, however, Western urban children appear to be developmentally delayed in this regard. Children who grow up constantly interacting with the natural world are much less likely to anthropomorphize other living things into late childhood.
Given that people living in WEIRD societies don’t routinely encounter or interact with animals other than humans or pets, it’s not surprising that they end up with a rather cartoonish understanding of the natural world. “Indeed,” the report concluded, “studying the cognitive development of folkbiology in urban children would seem the equivalent of studying ‘normal’ physical growth in malnourished children.”
During our dinner, I admitted to Heine, Henrich, and Norenzayan that the idea that I can only perceive reality through a distorted cultural lens was unnerving. For me the notion raised all sorts of metaphysical questions: Is my thinking so strange that I have little hope of understanding people from other cultures? Can I mold my own psyche or the psyches of my children to be less WEIRD and more able to think like the rest of the world? If I did, would I be happier?
Henrich reacted with mild concern that I was taking this research so personally. He had not intended, he told me, for his work to be read as postmodern self-help advice. “I think we’re really interested in these questions for the questions’ sake,” he said.
The three insisted that their goal was not to say that one culturally shaped psychology was better or worse than another—only that we’ll never truly understand human behavior and cognition until we expand the sample pool beyond its current small slice of humanity. Despite these assurances, however, I found it hard not to read a message between the lines of their research. When they write, for example, that weird children develop their understanding of the natural world in a “culturally and experientially impoverished environment” and that they are in this way the equivalent of “malnourished children,” it’s difficult to see this as a good thing.

THE TURN THAT HENRICH, Heine, and Norenzayan are asking social scientists to make is not an easy one: accounting for the influence of culture on cognition will be a herculean task. Cultures are not monolithic; they can be endlessly parsed. Ethnic backgrounds, religious beliefs, economic status, parenting styles, rural upbringing versus urban or suburban—there are hundreds of cultural differences that individually and in endless combinations influence our conceptions of fairness, how we categorize things, our method of judging and decision making, and our deeply held beliefs about the nature of the self, among other aspects of our psychological makeup.
We are just at the beginning of learning how these fine-grained cultural differences affect our thinking. Recent research has shown that people in “tight” cultures, those with strong norms and low tolerance for deviant behavior (think India, Malaysia, and Pakistan), develop higher impulse control and more self-monitoring abilities than those from other places. Men raised in the honor culture of the American South have been shown to experience much larger surges of testosterone after insults than do Northerners. Research published late last year suggested psychological differences at the city level too. Compared to San Franciscans, Bostonians’ internal sense of self-worth is more dependent on community status and financial and educational achievement. “A cultural difference doesn’t have to be big to be important,” Norenzayan said. “We’re not just talking about comparing New York yuppies to the Dani tribesmen of Papua New Guinea.”
As Norenzayan sees it, the last few generations of psychologists have suffered from “physics envy,” and they need to get over it. The job, experimental psychologists often assumed, was to push past the content of people’s thoughts and see the underlying universal hardware at work. “This is a deeply flawed way of studying human nature,” Norenzayan told me, “because the content of our thoughts and their process are intertwined.” In other words, if human cognition is shaped by cultural ideas and behavior, it can’t be studied without taking into account what those ideas and behaviors are and how they are different from place to place.
This new approach suggests the possibility of reverse-engineering psychological research: look at cultural content first; cognition and behavior second. Norenzayan’s recent work on religious belief is perhaps the best example of the intellectual landscape that is now open for study. When Norenzayan became a student of psychology in 1994, four years after his family had moved from Lebanon to America, he was excited to study the effect of religion on human psychology. “I remember opening textbook after textbook and turning to the index and looking for the word ‘religion,’ ” he told me, “Again and again the very word wouldn’t be listed. This was shocking. How could psychology be the science of human behavior and have nothing to say about religion? Where I grew up you’d have to be in a coma not to notice the importance of religion on how people perceive themselves and the world around them.”
Norenzayan became interested in how certain religious beliefs, handed down through generations, may have shaped human psychology to make possible the creation of large-scale societies. He has suggested that there may be a connection between the growth of religions that believe in “morally concerned deities”—that is, a god or gods who care if people are good or bad—and the evolution of large cities and nations. To be cooperative in large groups of relative strangers, in other words, might have required the shared belief that an all-powerful being was forever watching over your shoulder.
If religion was necessary in the development of large-scale societies, can large-scale societies survive without religion? Norenzayan points to parts of Scandinavia with atheist majorities that seem to be doing just fine. They may have climbed the ladder of religion and effectively kicked it away. Or perhaps, after a thousand years of religious belief, the idea of an unseen entity always watching your behavior remains in our culturally shaped thinking even after the belief in God dissipates or disappears.
Why, I asked Norenzayan, if religion might have been so central to human psychology, have researchers not delved into the topic? “Experimental psychologists are the weirdest of the weird,” said Norenzayan. “They are almost the least religious academics, next to biologists. And because academics mostly talk amongst themselves, they could look around and say, ‘No one who is important to me is religious, so this must not be very important.’” Indeed, almost every major theorist on human behavior in the last 100 years predicted that it was just a matter of time before religion was a vestige of the past. But the world persists in being a very religious place.

HENRICH, HEINE, AND NORENZAYAN’S FEAR of being ostracized after the publication of the WEIRD paper turned out to be misplaced. Response to the paper, both published and otherwise, has been nearly universally positive, with more than a few of their colleagues suggesting that the work will spark fundamental changes. “I have no doubt that this paper is going to change the social sciences,” said Richard Nisbett, an eminent psychologist at the University of Michigan. “It just puts it all in one place and makes such a bold statement.”
More remarkable still, after reading the paper, academics from other disciplines began to come forward with their own mea culpas. Commenting on the paper, two brain researchers from Northwestern University argued (pdf) that the nascent field of neuroimaging had made the same mistake as psychologists, noting that 90 percent of neuroimaging studies were performed in Western countries. Researchers in motor development similarly suggested that their discipline’s body of research ignored how different child-rearing practices around the world can dramatically influence states of development. Two psycholinguistics professors suggested that their colleagues had also made the same mistake: blithely assuming human homogeneity while focusing their research primarily on one rather small slice of humanity.
At its heart, the challenge of the WEIRD paper is not simply to the field of experimental human research (do more cross-cultural studies!); it is a challenge to our Western conception of human nature. For some time now, the most widely accepted answer to the question of why humans, among all animals, have so successfully adapted to environments across the globe is that we have big brains with the ability to learn, improvise, and problem-solve.
Henrich has challenged this “cognitive niche” hypothesis with the “cultural niche” hypothesis. He notes that the amount of knowledge in any culture is far greater than the capacity of individuals to learn or figure it all out on their own. He suggests that individuals tap that cultural storehouse of knowledge simply by mimicking (often unconsciously) the behavior and ways of thinking of those around them. We shape a tool in a certain manner, adhere to a food taboo, or think about fairness in a particular way, not because we individually have figured out that behavior’s adaptive value, but because we instinctively trust our culture to show us the way. When Henrich asked Fijian women why they avoided certain potentially toxic fish during pregnancy and breastfeeding, he found that many didn’t know or had fanciful reasons. Regardless of their personal understanding, by mimicking this culturally adaptive behavior they were protecting their offspring. The unique trick of human psychology, these researchers suggest, might be this: our big brains are evolved to let local culture lead us in life’s dance.
The applications of this new way of looking at the human mind are still in the offing. Henrich suggests that his research about fairness might first be applied to anyone working in international relations or development. People are not “plug and play,” as he puts it, and you cannot expect to drop a Western court system or form of government into another culture and expect it to work as it does back home. Those trying to use economic incentives to encourage sustainable land use will similarly need to understand local notions of fairness to have any chance of influencing behavior in predictable ways.
Because of our peculiarly Western way of thinking of ourselves as independent of others, this idea of the culturally shaped mind doesn’t go down very easily. Perhaps the richest and most established vein of cultural psychology—that which compares Western and Eastern concepts of the self—goes to the heart of this problem. Heine has spent much of his career following the lead of a seminal paper published in 1991 by Hazel Rose Markus, of Stanford University, and Shinobu Kitayama, who is now at the University of Michigan. Markus and Kitayama suggested that different cultures foster strikingly different views of the self, particularly along one axis: some cultures regard the self as independent from others; others see the self as interdependent. The interdependent self—which is more the norm in East Asian countries, including Japan and China—connects itself with others in a social group and favors social harmony over self-expression. The independent self—which is most prominent in America—focuses on individual attributes and preferences and thinks of the self as existing apart from the group.
The classic "rod and frame" task: Is the line in the center vertical?
The classic “rod and frame” task: Is the line in the center vertical?
That we in the West develop brains that are wired to see ourselves as separate from others may also be connected to differences in how we reason, Heine argues. Unlike the vast majority of the world, Westerners (and Americans in particular) tend to reason analytically as opposed to holistically. That is, the American mind strives to figure out the world by taking it apart and examining its pieces. Show a Japanese and an American the same cartoon of an aquarium, and the American will remember details mostly about the moving fish while the Japanese observer will likely later be able to describe the seaweed, the bubbles, and other objects in the background. Shown another way, in a different test analytic Americans will do better on something called the “rod and frame” task, where one has to judge whether a line is vertical even though the frame around it is skewed. Americans see the line as apart from the frame, just as they see themselves as apart from the group.
Heine and others suggest that such differences may be the echoes of cultural activities and trends going back thousands of years. Whether you think of yourself as interdependent or independent may depend on whether your distant ancestors farmed rice (which required a great deal of shared labor and group cooperation) or herded animals (which rewarded individualism and aggression). Heine points to Nisbett at Michigan, who has argued (pdf) that the analytic/holistic dichotomy in reasoning styles can be clearly seen, respectively, in Greek and Chinese philosophical writing dating back 2,500 years. These psychological trends and tendencies may echo down generations, hundreds of years after the activity or situation that brought them into existence has disappeared or fundamentally changed.
And here is the rub: the culturally shaped analytic/individualistic mind-sets may partly explain why Western researchers have so dramatically failed to take into account the interplay between culture and cognition. In the end, the goal of boiling down human psychology to hardwiring is not surprising given the type of mind that has been designing the studies. Taking an object (in this case the human mind) out of its context is, after all, what distinguishes the analytic reasoning style prevalent in the West. Similarly, we may have underestimated the impact of culture because the very ideas of being subject to the will of larger historical currents and of unconsciously mimicking the cognition of those around us challenges our Western conception of the self as independent and self-determined. The historical missteps of Western researchers, in other words, have been the predictable consequences of the WEIRD mind doing the thinking.
- www.psmag.com/

Comments

  • Al_G_Funguy

    If someone played the ultimatum game and had to split $1 million, and only gave me $50,000, I would certainly accept the "pittance" offered. If it were $100, and they only offered me $5, I might reject.
    Different individuals may have a varying ranking of preferences for receiving sums of money and for enforcing fairness.
  • Azzurra

    I'd just like to state my observation that, by engaging in the discussion of the ultimatum game, the commenters seem to be displaying the hallmark of the analytical, western mind - through singling out of specific information instead of paying more attention to the holistic implications of the article. (This is by no means a critique, just an observation.)
  • Steven

    Yes, I thought of the same issue, but the Machiguenga seemed to accept even the smallest amount possible, and it wasn't an individual variance but a group-wide variance from "our" (I presume we're both WEIRD) group. Not to mention there were some groups wherein people regularly rejected offers of 60% of the total. Really, the whole point is that this isn't about individual variance as much as there exists measurable group variance in cognition.
  • Bear Peterson

    I wonder how much do the Machiguenga normally share between each other in their normal lives? That may be a large factor here. It may be that the smallest pittance is simply token, if they know they would most like share in the other's windfall anyway, what difference who holds on to it?
  • Bryce Anderson

    That reasoning sounds implausible, since it would affect the behavior of both players. The same reasoning that would lead P2 to accept a lower offer would lead P1 to make a higher offer.
  • Gabe

    Unless a higher offer could be seen as rude or showing off.
  • Helga Vierich

    Yes, they do.
  • Bryce Anderson

    This example doesn't prove "measurable group variance in cognition." The differences seem to be entirely about the amount of money involved.
    As of 2001, no real Ultimatum Game experiment has been run with a pot of $100 or more, and $10 is a far more common amount. What, you think psychology researchers are made of money? http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/pa...
    And the absolute amount of the "minimum acceptable offer" drops from 38% of the pot to 28% of the pot as the size of the pot grows from $1 (a joke) to $80 (even a college student can afford to skip out on $20). How much lower could it go when the people involved are desperately poor and the amount of the pot rises to a week's wages?
    There's always the risk of overgeneralizing test results. But different experimental results between two groups doesn't always prove cultural differences.
  • the_count

    I was contemplating a similar dichotomy, but with opposite conclusions - if the amount the other person received was $100 and he only gave me $1, I would take it because the amounts of money involved for both of us are not life changing, it really would not be very meaningful either way. If the other person was getting $1 million and giving me only $1, I would totally reject that since that does involve a life-changing amount of money. I'm not sure what I'd do if offered $50,000, that's starting to get significant, but probably not strongly life-changing. A threshold exists somewhere, I don't know where. It would be really awesome if the game could be repeated among the Machiguenga using life changing amounts of money like that instead of such minor amounts. I have no idea if they would get a different result, but I'd like to know!
  • Lesley

    $50,000 would have a MASSIVE impact on my life as it would pay my student loan debt and honestly free me beyond what I can even comprehend. So even among WEIRD folks, what equates to a life changing amount can vary greatly.
  • NickDB

    But life saving amounts differ from place to place and culture to culture. $100 wouldn't change my life, but to someone in a slum in Calcutta or a village in the Amazon it would.
  • vinny Lohan

    I do not think $100 will have a profound long-term change in a person's life even in a slum in Calcutta. I could have a few decades ago, but not anymore.
  • Gaurav Ragtah

    Actually, yes it will. Most people below poverty line live on less than $1 or $2 a day.
  • mathsjames

    In that case $100 is about 2 to 3 months income for them which equates to about $5000 ish for the median westerner as we were debating whether $50,000 (i.e. a couple of years wages for a westerner) would be life changing I don't think you can call 3 months wages life changing or at least it is not obvious it would be.
  • lee

    If you're that poor, then it certainly is life changing - it means you don't have to go hungry (or have your kids go hungry) for a few more nights.
  • Jukka Aakula

    100$ is the kind of money people borrow from Kiva to make an investment for something which they then sell in their shop and get maybe 180$ and so one. So it is not only food for three months in India but it is CAPITAL for investment.
  • Nasaraajo

    Yes ... but you are ignoring the fact that the slum-dweller in Kolkata doesn't have your long-term view. Where I live, 24 hours is long-term.
  • Jeff Schmitz

    that is all the money in the world in a lot of parts of the world - yes in the US that's a happy hour bill - but not in a lot of places
  • Richard Wilson

    Um, in many parts of the US, Pine Ridge, to be extreme, 100 dollars is alot of moolah, esp. since the average annual wage is under 5,000. Suicide is 500-15,000 times the US or Canadian average for Indiians and Alaskan Natives.
  • missbstories

    I want to join you in the world where $50,000 isn't life-changing. With that plus my savings, I could pay off my student debt and live a completely different lifestyle. As is, I have to wait 10 years and watch my pursestrings carefully.
    It's also very strange to me that taking any amount of money is considered "selfish" ... isn't refusing a gift because the other person is getting more than you just about the most absurdly selfish thing one could do?
  • vinny Lohan

    I agreed with your point on life changing money, it really would be amazing to see what happens if life changing amounts of money was used.
    However a similar dichotomy would be to use what would be a small amount to the Machiguenga. Because I think that will be a better representation of what value an average american has for $100 (which is not a few days pay, as it was in the locals case).
    Then maybe we will start seeing people behaving more to the 50/50 way of sharing. As in this scenario they will have the fear of the other person refusing to accept a small amount of money, knowing the person giving will not get to keep any if they say no.
  • Jukka Aakula

    But Machiguenga ACCEPT almost always the offer when it is small money. If it is bigger probably they would accept also because it is more severe then.
    But maybe westerners would change the behavior if the money would be say 100 000 dollars and they would be offered 10 000 and the difference between Machiguenga and westerners would not be so big.
    But then Henrich et al also report on some other groups in New Guinea And Indonesia who always GIVE more than 50%. How would they maybe change their behaviour?
  • delta

    My instinct would be exactly the opposite: If someone got $1 million and was offering me $1, then I'd feel worse about refusing it because it was potentially life-changing for the other person (not so for just $100). So I'd wind up looking like the Machiguenga in that case. Of course, if the other person was some jerk billionaire then I'd refuse it without any qualms.
  • lee

    But why should they get 1 million when they aren't generous enough to give you any?
  • Scott McDaniel

    So you're making judgments not only on the hypothetical person not splitting evenly with you, but with how other people you don't even know deciding how they'd handle the hypothetical situation?
  • Vitaly Gashpar

    Just to put this into a different context, in King Solomon's dilemma, you'd be the one asking for the baby to be split in half.
  • Bryce Anderson

    This hypothetical tightwad would have their lives changed by $999,999, true. But not much more than their lives would have been changed by $700,000, or $500,000, or some other number that would have given you a fair shake as well.
    And yet this hypothetical tightwad offered you *nothing*. If he's that greedy and indifferent to the lives of others, how do you think he'll use the money once he gets it? Better to let the researchers keep it, and use it to collect more data.
  • chrisco

    Can we get some volunteers fund and participate in the million-dollar version of the study, please :)
  • Vitaly Gashpar

    Wait a minute, so you would screw someone out of having a million dollars simply because you couldn't get more $? Remember, you don't know the other person, have no connections or obligations to one another. Maybe you should query what is it in you that makes you wish to punish others for the things you aren't able to have?
    By the way, this is in no way intended to be a personal attack, but I think your thinking (or rather the source/cause of it) is what was at the crux of the experiment. I just find it fascinating.
  • Guest

    The other person has proven themselves unusually greedy and selfish. They've also done nothing to deserve having this fortune showered upon them. And yes, that did sound like a personal attack.
  • Pops

    You do know something about the other person, you know they're extraordinarily greedy and selfish. I, for one, would gladly part with $1 of my money to keep such a person from acquiring $999,999 that they've done absolutely nothing to deserve.
  • Thisisfake Fakeness

    This is incredible. The study highlights how often the weird mind zeros in on specifics and yall go ahead and do the same in your hypotheticals.
    What if the person needed the million to save a local hospital from bankruptcy? This was after a particularly bad rain storm and they had exhausted all other hope of help. You all have ignored the holistic backgrounds that might be attached to each individual and simply determined that keeping the money has something to do with a quality of the individual.
    Jesus, its right there in the article.
  • Pops

    And what if I'm the one who needed the million to save a local hospital from bankruptcy and the other guy is just going to waste it on slot machines and cocaine? Statistically speaking, him and I are equally (un)likely to have a selfless cause on which to spend that money in sight. If you're making stuff up to make me look bad, what's stopping me from making stuff up to make me look good? Fact is, I can only speculate on why this other person wouldn't offer a fair share of this fortune that fell into their lap through no merit of their own, and I find self-absorption a more likely explanation than them needing every last dollar in that fortune for completely altruistic reasons. And if you can't understand that, you probably think Bernie Madoff is innocent.
    And no, this is not what the article is about. Nowhere there is a 999,999-to-1 offer discussed.
    Now here's an offer from me: You stop trying to come off as enlightened and I abstain from telling you what you're really coming off as.
  • guest

    Who are we to judge if the other person is greedy or selfish just because they didn't give you more money than they were willing to give? To say such a thing would just be sulking. Imagine a little child crying "It's not fair!!" because he himself couldn't get a bigger piece of candy. How annoying is that?
    Who is to judge whether either player "deserves" any of the money? Since money is received for "free" on both sides, shouldn't they be content with whatever amount they receive? The concept of "fairness" arises from comparison. How do we determine if something is ultimately and absolutely fair or unfair?
    Also, if you decided to refuse the small amount of money to "punish" (the part of the game that I really don't understand) the other player, wouldn't that make you more selfish? What gave you the notion that you were better than the other person as to judge him to be "unworthy" of the money?
  • Pops

    Are you seriously not getting what this scenario is about. It's not the hand of God deciding that I would get $1 and this other guy $999,999. He didn't win that fortune at the lottery and it didn't fall into his lap from the sky. It's the other guy's decision to divide the money this way. Nobody gave him a single dime, in fact - a total of a million dollars was given *to us*. Every dollar he keeps to himself is a dollar he keeps from me. He could've decided to give me $10,000 and keep $990,000, and I'd still have called him an immense jerk but would've taken the money because that's 10 grand I could use. Instead he decided to give me the smallest possible amount he was allowed to. How in the world is that not selfish? I seriously think there must be something psychologically wrong with anyone who'd make a 999,999-to-1 offer. You want to talk annoying, what's annoying is anyone defending such an antisocial creep and telling me that I'm the one with a problem.
    I determine the concept of fairness with something called the Golden Rule: do unto others as you'd have them do unto you. The corollary to that is: ask of others as you'd have them ask of you. Were I in his shoes, I'd have offered half the sum and blushed at the thought of offering less. So right off the bat I have 2 things. I have the knowledge that I'm not more selfish than him, thank you very much. And I have a baseline from which to judge fairness, one by which this guy comes $499,999 short.
    Everybody is inherently entitled to judge anybody. The only qualification needed to pass judgment is the possession of a working brain. Even people with a chip on their shoulder about how they're too nice to judge others end up doing it, as you're doing to me. Rest assured that I've judged you too.
  • Fateh

    Thank you.
  • mtwzzyzx

    It WAS using, what were to them, life changing amounts of money- in a close to sustenance society, a few days wages are not to be sneered at:
    "The stakes Henrich used in the game with the Machiguenga were not insubstantial—roughly equivalent to the few days’ wages they sometimes earned from episodic work with logging or oil companies."
    which is why it's probably the worst study example in the article.
  • Bryce Anderson

    So, not just a few days' wages, but a few days of *very good* wages, doing work that's difficult, dangerous, and hard to come by.
  • Janet Bareiss

    There was a game show on 10 or so years ago, where there was a banker, a bunch of pretty girls with suitcases, The contestant picked a suitcase and then eliminated the rest disclosing the amounts that were in the others. The banker would make them an offer after each elimination. It generally came down to when it was a 'life-changing amount', If the banker never offered them the 'life changing amount' or they concluded based on elimination, that chances were highly in their favor that their suitcase had the 'life changing amount' of money, then they continued to reject the banker's offer of free money.
  • Steve Fahnestalk

    It was (and might still be) called "Deal or No Deal"--with Howie Mandel as the host. I watch little TV, but I'm surprised you don't know that.
  • ced

    you're assuming he's american who shares the same cultural background as you, therefore proving the point of the article :)
  • mightydoll

    you're assuming the OP is male, therefore proving the point of the article ;)
  • guest

    "he" can be a gender neutral pronoun. Unless you're one of those feminists.
  • Speechless

    the only gender neutral pronoun is 'their'.
  • BraveNewCurrency

    Does it make (economic) sense to refuse to take $5 if someone gives it to you for free?
  • Andy

    It sure does. People want to maximize utility.
    When someone pays $15 to watch a movie, he's saying the enjoyment of that movie is worth more than $15 to him.
    When someone refuse to take a $5 gift in an ultimatum game, he's saying the enjoyment of punishing the other person is worth more than $5 to him.
  • texan99

    Certainly, if you are looking at the bigger picture and expecting repeated interactions where signaling, reward, and punishment are important.
  • Nick

    However, not in contexts where there would be no likely repetition of interaction. In Game theory if you knew this game was game one of x games always played with the same other player, or y players who will know what was done and what resulted, then it makes sense to reject or you are open to abuse next time as people will think you always take the weak offer. However, in a situation where there is only this one game (or there is the possibility of playing again some other time with unrelated players) unless the offer is zero there is no logical reason for rejecting the money.
    Traditional economists have taken the game to represent the former scenario and to paraphrase real life commercial interactions between strangers, and this is a big leap. However, the people doing the study may have also been making this leap, it's unclear what was governing their thinking when they made decisions.
  • ced

    You're a victim of the same cognitive bias detailed in the article. You're measuring utility based on whether or not the game will be played multiple times. When you do that, you assume that the goal of the person rejecting the money is simply to maximize the amount of money received. If you approached the question with what the authors describe as a holistic mindset, it's easier to see that the perceived value of teaching the other person a lesson about sharing and fairness can exceed the $5, even if the game is only played once, and even if the other person remains anonymous.
  • Bryce Anderson

    One of the main features of the Ultimatum Game is that it's a one-time random interaction, with no future interactions expected. But human beings generally behave as though they expect future interactions, possibly because in our evolutionary past, repeated interaction was the norm.
    In other words, if the players were behaving in an economically rational way, they would not be looking at a "bigger picture" where such interactions were expected.

Book

Nema komentara:

Objavi komentar